Lexical Event Ordering with an Edge-Factored Model - Informatics

Loading...

Lexical Event Ordering with an Edge-Factored Model

Omri Abend, Shay B. Cohen and Mark Steedman School of Informatics, University of Edinburgh Edinburgh EH8 9AB, United Kingdom {oabend,scohen,steedman}@inf.ed.ac.uk

Abstract Extensive lexical knowledge is necessary for temporal analysis and planning tasks. We address in this paper a lexical setting that allows for the straightforward incorporation of rich features and structural constraints. We explore a lexical event ordering task, namely determining the likely temporal order of events based solely on the identity of their predicates and arguments. We propose an “edgefactored” model for the task that decomposes over the edges of the event graph. We learn it using the structured perceptron. As lexical tasks require large amounts of text, we do not attempt manual annotation and instead use the textual order of events in a domain where this order is aligned with their temporal order, namely cooking recipes.

1

Introduction

Temporal relations between events are often implicit, and inferring them relies on lexical and world knowledge about the likely order of events. For instance, to execute the instruction “fry the onion,” the hearer should probably obtain oil beforehand, even if not instructed so explicitly. Lexical knowledge about the likely order of events is therefore necessary for any semantic task that requires temporal reasoning or planning, such as classifying temporal relations (Mani et al., 2006; Lapata and Lascarides, 2006; Yoshikawa et al., 2009; D’Souza and Ng, 2013; Mirza and Tonelli, 2014, inter alia), textual entailment (Dagan et al., 2013) or temporal information extraction (Ling and Weld, 2010). Lexical temporal knowledge is further important for model-

ing grammatical phenomena such as tense and aspect (Steedman, 2002). In this paper we address the task of lexical event ordering, namely predicting the ordering of events based only on the identity of the words comprising their predicates and arguments. Concretely, the task is to predict the order of an unordered set of predicate-argument structures. Predicting the likely order of event types is a step towards more intricate planning and reasoning scenarios (see §3), and is useful in itself for tasks such as conceptto-text generation (Reiter et al., 2000), or in validating the correctness of instruction sets. A related idea can be found in modeling sentence coherence (Lapata, 2003; Barzilay and Lapata, 2008, inter alia), although here we focus on lexical relations between events, rather than coherence relations between complete sentences. Compiling a resource of temporal tendencies between events can hardly be done manually, given the number and wealth of phenomena that have to be accounted for. Temporally annotated corpora, often annotated according to TimeML principles (Pustejovsky et al., 2003), are a useful resource for studying temporal relations. However, due to incurred costs, annotated corpora are too small for most lexical tasks. For instance, the TimeML annotated data used in the latest TempEval shared task contains only 100K words or so (UzZaman et al., 2013). Previous work that does not rely on manually annotated data has had some success in discovering temporal lexical relations between predicates (Chklovski and Pantel, 2004; Chambers and Jurafsky, 2008b; Talukdar et al., 2012). However, despite their appeal, these methods have mostly fo-

cused on inducing simple event types, consisting of single words (e.g., “buy-own”) or fixed expressions, and are hard to extend to include rich features (e.g., order-based and pattern-based features). Furthermore, measuring recall without annotated data is notoriously difficult, and evaluation is often precisionbased or extrinsic. We take a graph-based structured prediction approach to the task, motivated by the flexibility it allows in incorporating various feature sets and constraints. We use an edge-factored model, which decomposes over the edges in the graph of events comprising the recipe (§4). We estimate the model using the structured perceptron algorithm. We compare the structured perceptron approach to an approximate greedy baseline and to a locally normalized model reminiscent of common approaches for order learning, obtaining superior results (§8). The learning algorithm is of potential use in other ordering tasks such as machine translation reordering (Tromble and Eisner, 2009). We focus on domains in which the order of events in the text is aligned with their temporal order. By doing so we avoid the costly and error-prone manual annotation of temporal relations by using the textual order of recipes to approximate their temporal order.1 Specifically, we address the cooking recipes domain, which we motivate in §2. In summary, the contribution of this paper is three-fold: (1) we explore the task of lexical event ordering and means for its evaluation; (2) we present an edge-factored model for the task, and show it can be used to predict the order of events well (77.7% according to standard measures for ordering evaluation); (3) we present a method for extracting events and create a dataset of ordered events using recipes extracted from the web.

2

Related Work

Temporal semantics is receiving increasing attention in recent years. Lexical features are in frequent use and rely in most part on external resources which are either manually compiled or automatically induced. The line of work most closely related to ours focuses on inducing lexical relations between 1

See Cassidy et al. (2014) for a discussion of inter-annotator agreement in TimeML-based schemes.

event types. Most work has been unsupervised, often using pattern-based approaches relying on manually crafted (Chklovski and Pantel, 2004) or induced patterns (Davidov et al., 2007), that correlate with temporal relations (e.g., temporal discourse connectives). Talukdar et al. (2012) uses the textual order of events in Wikipedia biographical articles to induce lexical information. We use both textual order and discourse connectives to define our feature set, and explore a setting which allows for the straightforward incorporation of additional features. Chambers and Jurafsky (2008b; 2009) addressed the unsupervised induction of partially ordered event chains (or schema) in the news domain, centered around a common protagonist. One of their evaluation scenarios tackles a binary classification related to event ordering, and seeks to distinguish ordered sets of events from randomly permuted ones, yielding an accuracy of 75%. Manshadi et al. (2008) used language models to learn event sequences and conducted a similar evaluation on weblogs with about 65% accuracy. The classification task we explore here is considerably more complex (see §8). The task of script knowledge induction has been frequently addressed in recent years. Balasubramanian et al. (2013) and Pichotta and Mooney (2014) extended Chambers and Jurafsky’s model to include events that have multiple arguments. Jans et al. (2012) use skip-grams to capture event-event relations between not necessarily consecutive events. Regneri et al. (2010) constructed a temporal lexical knowledge base through crowd-sourcing. Their approach is appealing as it greatly reduces the costs incurred by manual annotation and can potentially be used in conjunction with lexical information obtained from raw text. Modi and Titov (2014) jointly learns the stereotypical order of events and their distributional representation, in order to capture paraphrased instances of the same event type. Frermann et al. (2014) models the joint task of inducing event paraphrases and their order using a Bayesian framework. All latter three works evaluated their induced temporal ordering knowledge on a binary prediction of whether a temporal relation between a pair of (not necessarily related) events holds, and not on the prediction of a complete permutation given an unordered event set as in this work. Their evaluation was conducted on 30 event pairs manually an-

notated through crowd-sourcing, where Modi and Titov (2014) further included an evaluation on a large set of pairs automatically extracted from the Gigaword corpus. The appeal of the cooking domain for studying various semantic phenomena has been recognized by several studies in NLP and AI (Tasse and Smith, 2008; Bollini et al., 2013; Cimiano et al., 2013; Regneri et al., 2013; Malmaud et al., 2014). The domain is here motivated by several considerations. First, recipes mostly describe concrete actions, rather than abstract relations, which are less relevant to temporal ordering. Second, from a practical point of view, many recipes are available online in computerreadable format. Third, the restrictiveness of the cooking domain can also be seen as an advantage, as it can reveal major conceptual challenges raised by the task, without introducing additional confounds.

3

Temporally Ordering Lexical Events

We formalize our task as follows. Let U be a set of event types, namely actions or states (represented as predicates) and objects which these actions operate on (represented as arguments to the predicates; mostly ingredients or kitchenware). Formally, each e ∈ U is a tuple ha, c1 , . . . , cn i where a is the main verb or predicate describing the event (such as “stir” or “mix”) and c1 , . . . , cn is a list of arguments that the predicate takes (e.g., “salt” or “spoon”). Two additional marked events, s and f , correspond to “start” and “finish” events. A recipe is a sequence of events in U , starting at s and ending at f . Given a recipe R = he1 , ..., em i, we wish to predict the order of the events just from the (multi)set {ei }m i=1 . In this work we use the textual order of events to approximate their temporal order (see, e.g., Talukdar et al. (2012) for a similar assumption). The validity of this assumption for cooking recipes is supported in §6. Figure 1 gives an example of a set of events extracted from our dataset for the dish “Apple Crisp Ala [sic] Brigitte.” Lexical information places quite a few limitations on the order of this recipe. For instance, in most cases serving is carried out at the end while putting the ingredients in is done prior to baking them. However, lexical knowledge in itself is unlikely to predict the exact ordering of the events

as given in the recipe (e.g., spreading butter might be done before or after baking). One of the major obstacles in tackling planning problems in AI is the knowledge bottleneck. Lexical event ordering is therefore a step towards more ambitious goals in planning. For instance, temporal relations may be used to induce planning operators (Mour˜ao et al., 2012), which can in turn be used to generate a plan (recipe) given a specified goal and an initial set of ingredients.

4

Model, Inference and Learning

In this section we describe the main learning components that compose our approach to event ordering. 4.1

Edge-Factored Model for Event Ordering

We hereby detail the linear model we use for ordering events. Let S = {e1 , . . . , em } ⊆ U be a set of events as mentioned in §3. Let G(S) = (S ∪ {s, f }, E(S)) be an almost-complete directed graph with E(S) = (S ∪{s})×(S ∪{f }) ⊆ U ×U . Every Hamiltonian path2 in G(S) that starts in s and ends in f defines an ordering of the events in S. The edge (ei , ej ) in such a path denotes that ei is the event that comes before ej . The modeling problem is to score Hamiltonian paths in a given directed graph G(S). Here, we use an edge-factored model. Let φ : (U × U ) → Rd be a feature function for pairs of events, represented as directed edges. In addition, let θ ∈ Rd be a weight vector. We define the score of a Hamiltonian path h = (h1 , . . . , hm+1 ) (hi ∈ E(S)) as:

score(h|S) =

m+1 X

θ> φ(hi )

(1)

i=1

Given a weight vector θ and a set of events S, inference is carried out by computing the highest scoring Hamiltonian path in G(S): h∗ = arg max score(h|S) h∈H(S)

(2)

where H(S) is the set of Hamiltonian paths in G(S) that start with s and end with f . The path h∗ is the best temporal ordering of the set of events S according to the model in Eq. 1 with weight vector θ. 2

A path in a graph that visits all nodes exactly once.

(a) e1 = hmix, ✏, tarragon, vinegari (a) Butter a deep baking dish, e2 =(b) hblend, ✏, mustardi e1 = hbutter, dishi put apples, flour, sugar e3 = hmix, ✏, salt,water, pepperi (a) apples, water, ... (b) Butter ait.deep baking dish, put e2 = hput, and cinnamon in Mix withcreami e4 = hblend, ✏, mayonnaise, sour e1 = flour, hbutter,cinnamon, dishi apples, water, flour, sugar itie =spoon and✏ispread butter and saltand cinhcover, e2 = hput, apples, water, ...5 namon it. MixBake with spoon and e3 = hmix, spoon, i over the at e6 =iti hchill, ✏iapplein mix. flour, cinnamon, spread butter and salt over the ape4 = hspread, butter, salt, mixi 350 degrees F until the apples e3 = hmix, with spoon, i ple mix. Bake at 350 degrees F until e5 = hbake, e4 =Fi hspread, butter, salt, ... are tender and the crust brown, the minutes. apples are Serve tender and e6 = hserve, cream, creami about 30 withthe crust over mixi e5 = hbake, Fi e6 = hserve, cream, creami

brown, about 30 minutes. cream or whipped cream.

with cream or whipped cream.

Serve

(b) you mix the tarragon and vinegar together and blend in the mustard. you mix in the salt and (c) pepper, blending well. you blendein the mayon1 naise and then the sour cream. you covere5 and s chill. e3

e6

(c)

e1 e2

s f

e3

e4

Figure 1: (a) The sequence of events representing the recipe for the dish “Apple Crisp Ala [sic] Brigitte.” (b) The actual recipe for this dish. (c) A complete Figure graph over the Example set of events with start and finishastates. in the graph is one of the 1: (a) of events describing recipeEach for internal the dishnode “.” (b) The actual recipe for this dish. (c) A c events ei for i ∈ {1, . . . , 6}. graph The path in blue bold denotes the correct Hamiltonian path describing the set of actions as ordered over the set of events with start and end states. Each internal node in the graphinis one of the even Red edges denote from start state and to“Apple the endCrisp state. Ala The [sic] in practice, arebrevity, weighted. Figure 1:the(a)recipe. Example of events describing for the dishin Brigitte.” For iedges 2 {1, .a. recipe .the , 5}. The path bold denotes theedges, correct Hamiltonian path arguments describing the set of actions that ne are represented as headwords and taken their syntactic type is omitted. (b) The actual recipe for this dish. (c) A complete to follow the recipe. graph over the set of events with start and end states. Each internal node in the graph is one of the events ei for 4.3pathLearning i 2 {1, .4.2 . . , 6}.Inference The path in blue bold denotes the correct Hamiltonian describing the set of actions as ordered in the recipe. Red edges denote edges3from the startInference state and to and the end state. The edges, in practice, Model, Learning The learning problem takesare as weighted. input a dataset con-

As mentioned above, inference with the edge- sisting of unordered sets of events, paired with a tar-m+1 X this section we describe the main learning compofactored model requiresInperformance solving the maximization get ordering. We consider two typesscore(h|S) of learning=al✓> (hi ) ILP formulation yields superior to the nents that compose our approach to event ordering. problem in Eq. 2. This corresponds to finding a gorithms for theX edge-factored model in the previousi=1 other evaluated systems (§8). ILP has been proven to Hamiltonian path in a complete graph, which is genZ(✓, S, e ) = (e1 , e) section. The first learnsexp in a✓> global training setting 1 be a practical and flexible tool in various structured Given a weight vector w and a set of even e : (e ,e)2E(S) erally an NP-hard problem. Reasonable approxima1 using the averaged structured perceptron (Collins, prediction tasks in NLP (Roth3.1 and Edge-Factored tau Yih, 2007; Model for Event Ordering is carried out by the computing the highe where e2002), S [ the {s, decoding f }. Thisference isalgorithm a locally normaltions for 2012; this problem NP-hard. Still tech1 , e2 2with being either Talukdar et al., Scaria etare al.,also 2013). Our ILP We now turn to explain the linear model we use for ing Hamiltonian path in model that(henceforth, gives the probability ofRC), G(S): niques are developed for specialized cases, dueized to log-linear one based on ILP G LOBAL -P or formulation is given in Appendix A. ordering events in time. Let S = {v , . . . , v } ✓ U 1 m transitioning to node e from node e . Maximizing the problem’s importance in discrete optimization. 2 1 the greedy one (G REEDY-P RC). Given a training inWe experiment with an additional infer-as mentioned be a setgreedy of events section 2. Let label hcof, the the score ininEq. hasits ancorrect interpretation finding the perS 1and structured ence algorithm, to the one described Despitesimilar its theoretical NP-hardness, this maxi- bestance h⇤ = arg max score(h|S) G(S) = (S [by {s,Lae}, E(S)) an almost-complete highestceptron scoringcalls paththe according an edge-factored inferencetoprocedure as a subroutine h2H(S) pata (2003) for sentence The algorithm it-E(S) mization problemordering. can be represented an Integer directed graph as with = (Sand [{s})⇥(S [{e}) ✓ Markovian model, such that: updates the weight vector θ according to the diferatively selects an outgoing (starting from the Linear Program (ILP),edge and using generic m+1 (U then ⇥ Usolved ). Every Hamiltonian path5 in G(S) thatvalue where is the set of on Hamiltonian paths Y ference between the of theH(S) feature function node s)techniques that has thefor largest to a node that has ILP weight optimization. Due to the relaPi |ethat p(h|✓, of S) as = an orp(e ✓, S),with s and end with e. h⇤ is the b i 1 , start starts in s and ends in e can bethe thought predicted path ( h∗ φ(hi )) and on the correct i=2 not beentively visited so length far, until all dering vertices covered, short of recipes (13.8 events on average Pi , vj ) in ofare the events in S. The path edge((v such poral ordering of the set of events S accordin φ(h )). c i where h = (h , . . . , h 1h m+1 ) is a Hamiltonian path at whichinpoint the paththe terminates by to fv. solved our corpus), problem cantravelling be effectively a path denotes that that second comes before structured model Equation 1 with weigh i is the event The learning tryin with hi = (ei 1 , ei ) being aalgorithm directed we edge inis based in most cases. 4.3 Learning vj . w.algorithm maximizes the on factored training. This the path. Initial experimentation suggested that The learning problem takes as input a dataset conThe modeling problem, therefore, is to score The proposed algorithmic setting is appealinggreedy for likelihood of a conditional log-linear model inference (henceforth, 3.2 G REEDY -L OG L IN)p: Inference sisting of of linear events, pairedformulation with a tar-inallows Hamiltonian paths a works given directed graph G(S) its unordered flexibility.sets The score better in practice than the ILP formulation get ordering. We rich consider two such types ofusing learning As mentioned above, inference with the us to use features, whileas ILP alallows the above. Here, an edge-factoredmodel.  fortowe the use locally-normalized Weθ>therefore do exp φ(e , e ) d gorithms for the edge-factored model in the previous factored model we presented would have to s 1 2 easily incorporate structural constraints. ILP model. Let Indeed, : (U ⇥not U )report ! results R be a feature p(eon = with this log2 |eglobal 1 , θ, S)inference section.has The first learns in a global training setting maximization problem in Eq. 2. This corr Z(θ, S, e1 ) been proven valuablevector in various NLP tasks (Roth for pairs of events, as directed linearrepresented model. G REEDY -L OG L IN closely resembles using the structured perceptron (Collins, to finding an Hamiltonian path in a complet andaveraged Yih, 2007; Talukdaredges. et al., In 2012; Scarialet etthe al., addition, ✓ 2 Rd bemodel a weight vec- (2003), learning of Lapata except that it is 2002), with the decoding algorithm being either the which is generally an NP-hard problem6 . In   2013). See Appendix A tor. for our ILP we formulation. X Then, define the score of an Hamiltonian a discriminative log-linear model, rather of a>generone based on ILP (henceforth,path G LOBAL ), or Z(θ, eE(S) expthere θ φ(e e) eral case is no1 , reasonable approximatio 1 ) = for h = -P (hRC ) (where hi S, 2model. 1 , . . . , hm+1 ative Markovian a baseline, experiment an additional the greedyAs one (G REEDYwe -P RC ).i 2 {1, . . with e : (e ,e)∈E(S) rithm to solve the maximization algorithm, a 1 . , m + 1}) as: algorithm,we similar the one 5de- The Feature Set The greedy second inference learning algorithm try istobased 6 The NP scribed by Lapata for sentence ordering. The 1where e1 ,all e2 the ∈ Scomplete ∪ {s, f }.setThis is acomplete locally normal-of finding a Hamilton Table presents of features usedproblem 5 on factored training. This(2003) algorithm maximizes the An Hamiltonian path in a graph is a path that visits all in an undirected graph can be trivially reduced to fi algorithm iterativelylog-linear selects anmodel outgoing ized log-linear that gives theconsider probability of for defining the featuremodel function . We likelihood of a conditional p: edge (startnodes exactly once. maximal Hamiltonian cycle in a directed graph. ing from the node s) that has the largest weight to transitioning to node e from node e 2 encodes the 1 . Maximizing three sets of features: Lexical writexp ✓> (e1 , e2 ) p(e2 |ea1node , ✓, S)that = has not been visited so far, until all ten ver-forms the of score Eq. 1pair has predicates an interpretation of finding the theinevent and objects; Z(✓, S, e1 ) tices are covered, at which point the path terminates highest scoring path according to an edge-factored by traveling to f . Markovian model, such that:

p(ei |ei−1 , θ, S),

Lexical

p(h|θ, S) =

m+1 Y

5

The Feature Set

Table 1 presents the complete set of features. We consider three sets of features: Lexical encodes the written forms of the event pair predicates and objects; Brown uses Brown clusters (Brown et al., 1992) to encode similar information, but allows generalization between distributionally similar words; and Frequency encodes the empirical distribution of temporally-related phenomena. The feature definitions make use of several functions. For brevity, we sometimes say that an event e is (a, c1 ) if e’s predicate is a and its first argument is c1 , disregarding its other arguments. Let C be a reference corpus of recipes for collecting statistics. The function B(w) gives the Brown cluster of a word w, as determined by clustering C into 50 clusters {1, . . . , 50}. The function ORD(a, c) returns the mean ordinal number of an (a, c) event in C. The ordinal number of the event ei in a recipe m (e1 , ..., em ) is defined as i − . 2

Frequency

where h = (h1 , . . . , hm+1 ) is a Hamiltonian path with hi = (ei−1 , ei ) being a directed edge in the path. Initial experimentation suggested that greedy inference (henceforth, G REEDY-L OG L IN) works better in practice than the ILP formulation for the locally-normalized model. We therefore do not report results on global inference with this log-linear model. We suspect that greedy inference works better with the log-linear model because it is trained locally, while the perceptron algorithm includes a global inference step in its training, and therefore better matches global decoding. G REEDY-L OG L IN closely resembles the learning model of Lapata (2003), as both are firstorder Markovian and use the same (greedy) inference procedure. Lapata’s model differs from G REEDY-L OG L IN in being a generative model, where each event is a tuple of features, and the transition probability between events is defined as the product of transition probabilities between feature pairs. G REEDY-L OG L IN is discriminative, so to be maximally comparable to the presented model.

Brown

i=1

Template (a1 , a2 ) (a1 , c21 ) (a2 , c11 ) (c11 , c21 ) (B(a1 ),B(a2 )) ∀(k, k0 ) ∈ K. |{(c1i , c2j ) : B(c1i ) = k, B(c2j ) = k0 }| ∀k ∈ K. |{c2i : B(c2i ) = k}| B(a2 ) ∀` ∈ L. log( + P` [(a2 , c21 )|(a1 , c11 )]) ∀` ∈ L. PMI` ((a1 , c11 ), (a2 , c21 )) ∀` ∈ L. PMI` ((a2 , c21 ), (a1 , c11 )) ORD(a2 , c21 )

Example (fry, add) (fry, oil) (onions, add) (onions, oil) (1,3) (5,4) : 2 12 : 1 5 -2.3 3.1 -2.0 3.2

Table 1: Feature templates used for computing φ. The templates operate on two events ha1 , c11 , . . . , c1m1 i and ha2 , c21 , . . . , c2m2 i. B(w) maps a word w to its Brown cluster in K = {1, . . . , 50}. ORD(e) returns the mean ordinal value of e. Feature templates which start with ∀ stand for multiple features, one for each element in the set quantified over. Non-numerical feature templates correspond to binary features. E.g., (fry, add) as an instance of (a1 , a2 ) is a binary feature indicating the appearance of an event with a1 = fry on one end of the edge and an event with a2 = add on the other end of it.  = 10−3 in our experiments. See text for elaboration.

We further encode the tendency of two events to appear with temporal discourse connectives, such as “before” or “until.” We define a linkage between two events as a triplet (e1 , e2 , `) ∈ (U × U × L), where L is the set of linkage types, defined according to their marker’s written form. §6 details the extraction process of linkages from recipes. We further include a special linkage type linear based on the order of events in the text, and consider every pair of events e1 and e2 that follow one another in a recipe as linked under this linkage type. For each linkage type ` ∈ L, we define an empirical probability distribution P` ((a, c1 ), (a0 , c01 )) = P ((a, c1 ), (a0 , c01 )|`), based on simple counting. The function PMI gives the point-wise mutual information of two events and is defined as: 0

0

PMI` ((a, c), (a , c )) = log



P` ((a, c1 ), (a0 , c01 )) P` (a, c1 ) · P` (a0 , c01 )



Frequency-based features encode the empirical estimate of the probabilities that various pairs of features would occur one after the other or linked with a discourse marker. They are equivalent to using probabilities extracted from maximum likelihood estima-

tion according to a bigram model in the discriminative learning. While some of this information is implicitly found in the lexical features, collecting frequency counts from a large training set is much quicker than running costly structured optimization. Rather the discriminative training can weigh the different empirical probabilities according to their discriminative power. Indeed we find that these features are important in practice and can result in high accuracy even after training on a small training set.

6

The Recipe Dataset

Data and Preprocessing. The data is extracted from a recipe repository found on the web.3 The recipes are given as free text. To extract event types we run the Stanford CoreNLP4 pipeline of a tokenizer, POS tagger, a lexical constituency parser (the englishPCFG parsing model) and extract typed Stanford dependencies (de Marneffe and Manning, 2008). As is common with web extractions, the recipes contain occasional spelling, grammatical and formatting errors. The corpus consists of 139 files, 73484 recipes, 1.02M events (13.8 events per recipe on average) and 11.05M words.5 Event Extraction. We focus on verbal events and do not extract nominal and adjectival argument structures, which are not as well supported by current parsing technology. Any verb is taken to define an event, aside from modal verbs, auxiliaries and secondary verbs. A secondary verb (e.g., “let,” “begin”) does not describe an action in its own right, but rather modifies an event introduced by another verb. We identify these verbs heuristically using a list given in Dixon (2005, p. 490–491) and a few simple rules defined over parse trees. E.g., from the sentence “you should begin to chop the onion,” we extract a single event with a predicate “chop.” Arguments are taken to be the immediate dependents of the predicate that have an argument dependency type (such as direct or indirect objects) according to the extracted Stanford dependencies. For prepositional phrases, we include the preposition as part of 3 4 5

the argument. Argument indices are determined by their order in the text. The order of events is taken to be the order of their verbs in the text. Linkage Extraction. We focus on a subset of linkage relations, which are relevant for temporal relations. We use Pitler and Nenkova’s (2009) explicit discourse connectives classifier to identify temporal discourse linkers, discarding all other discourse linkers. Once a discourse linker has been detected, we heuristically extract its arguments (namely the pair of verbs it links) according to a deterministic extraction rule defined over the parse tree. We find 28 distinct connectives in our training set, where the 5 most common linkers “until,” “then,” “before,” “when” and “as” cover over 95% of the instances. We extract 36756 such linkages from the corpus, 0.5 linkages per recipe on average. Temporal and Textual Ordering. In order to confirm that temporal and textual order of recipes are generally in agreement, we manually examine the first 20 recipes in our development set. One recipe was excluded as noise6 , resulting in 19 recipes and 353 events. We identify the sources of misalignment between the linear order and the temporal order of the events.7 13 events (3.7%) did not have any clear temporal orderings. These consisted of mostly negations and modalities (e.g., “do not overbrown!”), sub-section headings (e.g., “Preparation”) or other general statements that do not constitute actions or states. For the remaining 340 events, we compare their linear and the temporal orderings. We estimate the frequency of sub-sequences that contradict the temporal order and confirm that they occur only infrequently. We find that most disagreements fall into these two categories: (1) disjunctions between several events, only one of which will actually take place (e.g., “roll Springerle pin over dough, or press mold into top”); (2) a pair, or less commonly a triplet, of events are expressed in reverse order. For instance, “place on greased and floured cookie sheet,” where greasing and flouring should occur before the placing action. We note that assuming the alignment of the temporal and textual order

http://www.ffts.com/recipes.htm http://nlp.stanford.edu/software/corenlp.shtml

Links to the original recipes, the preprocessed recipes and all extracted events can be found in http://homepages. inf.ed.ac.uk/oabend/event_order.html.

6

This did not result from an extraction problem, but rather from the recipe text itself being too noisy to interpret. 7 Events are parsed manually so to avoid confounding the results with the parser’s performance.

of recipes does not suggest that the textual order is the only order of events that would yield the same outcome. We compute the Kendall’s Tau correlation, a standard measure for information ordering (Lapata, 2006), between the temporal and linear orderings for each recipe. In cases of several events that happen simultaneously (including disjunctions), we take their ordinals to be equal. For instance, for three events where the last two happen at the same time, we take their ordering to be (1,2,2) in our analysis. We find that indeed temporal and textual orderings are in very high agreement, with 6 recipes of the 19 perfectly aligned. The average Kendall’s Tau between the temporal ordering and the linear one is 0.924.

7

Experimental Setup

Evaluation. We compute the accuracy of our algorithms by comparing the predicted order to the one in which the events are written. We first compute the number of exact matches, denoted with E XACT, namely the percentage of recipes in which the predicted and the textual orders are the same. For a more detailed analysis of imperfect predictions, we compute the agreement between subsequences of the orderings. We borrow the notion of a “concordant pair” from the definition of Kendall’s Tau and generalize it to capture agreement of longer sub-sequences. Two k-tuples of integers (x1 , ..., xk ) and (y1 , ..., yk ) are said to “agree in order” if for every 1 ≤ i < j ≤ k, xi < xj iff yi < yj . Given two orderings of the same recipe O1 = (eτ (1) , ..., eτ (m) ) and O2 = (eσ(1) , ..., eσ(m) ) (where τ and σ are permutations over [m] = {1, . . . , m}) and given a sequence of k monotonically increasing indices t = (i1 , ..., ik ), t is said to be a “concordant k-tuple” of O1 and O2 if (τ (i1 ), ..., τ (ik )) and (σ(i1 ), ..., σ(ik )) agree in order, as defined above. Denote the unordered recipes of the test data as i i {Ri }N i=1 , where Ri = {e1 , ..., emi } ⊂ U for all i, and their target orderings Σ = {σi }N i=1 , where σi is a permutation over [mi ]. Assume we wish to evaluate a set of predicted orderings for this test data T = {τi }N i=1 , where again τi is a permutation over [mi ]. Denote the number of concordant k-tuples of σi and τi as conc(σi , τi ). The total number of of

 monotonically increasing k-tuples of indices is mki . The k-wise (micro-averaged) accuracy of T with respect to Σ is: PN acck (Σ, T) =

conc(σi , τi ) PN mi 

i=1

i=1

k

Any k-tuples containing the start node s or the end node f are excluded, as their ordering is trivial. Recipes of length less than k are discarded when computing acck . A micro-averaged accuracy measure is used so as not to disproportionately weigh short recipes. However, in order to allow comparison to mean Kendall’s Tau, commonly used in works on order learning, we further report a macroaveraged acc2 by computing acc2 for each recipe separately, and taking the average of resulting accuracy levels. Average Kendall’s Tau can now be computed by 2acc2 − 1 for the macro-averaged acc2 score. Data. We randomly partition the text into training, test and development sets, taking an 80-10-10 percent split. We do not partition the individual files so as to avoid statistical artifacts introduced by recipe duplications or near-duplications. The training, development and test sets contain 58038, 7667 and 7779 recipes respectively. The total number of feature template instantiations in the training data is 8.94M. Baselines and Algorithms. We compare three learning algorithms. G LOBAL -P RC is the structured perceptron algorithm that uses ILP inference. G REEDY-P RC is a structured perceptron in which inference is done greedily. G REEDY-L OG L IN is the locally normalized log-linear model with greedy inference. R ANDOM randomly (uniformly) selects a permutation of the recipe’s events. Experimental Settings. The structured perceptron algorithms, G LOBAL -P RC and G REEDY-P RC, are run with a learning rate of 0.1 for 3 iterations. To avoid exceedingly long runs, we set a time limit in seconds β on the running time of each ILP inference stage used in G LOBAL -P RC. We consider two training scenarios: 4K, which trains on the first 4K recipes of the training set, and 58K, which trains on the full training data of 58K recipes. In G LOBAL -P RC we set β to be 30 seconds for the 4K

58K

4K

Alg. G LOBAL -P RC G REEDY-P RC G REEDY-L OG L IN G LOBAL -P RC G REEDY-P RC G REEDY-L OG L IN R ANDOM

acc2 MI MA 71.2 77.7 60.8 68.0 65.6 71.5 68.9 76.4 60.7 67.8 66.3 72.4 50.0 51.2

acc3

acc4

E XACT

44.7 30.6 35.8 41.3 30.6 36.6 16.7

27.9 15.0 18.7 24.8 15.2 19.4 4.2

35.1 20.4 21.0 34.4 20.5 21.3 0.5

percents. Columns correspond to evaluation measures, namely accuracy of sub-sequences of lengths 2 (micro and macro averages), 3 and 4, and exact match. The upper (lower) part presents results for a training set of 4K (58K) samples. G LOBAL -P RC is run with β = 30 for 4K and with β = 5 for 58K. In 58K, all models are run with the Full feature set. In 4K, following prior experimentation on the development set, we select the best performing feature sets (Full for G REEDY-L OG L IN and G REEDY-P RC; Fr + Lex for G LOBAL -P RC).

scenario, and 5 seconds in the 58K scenario. The number of threads was limited to 3. Where the time limit is reached before an optimal solution is found, the highest scoring Hamiltonian path found up to that point is returned by the ILP solver. In the infrequent samples where no feasible solution is found during training, the sample is skipped over, while at test time, we perform greedy inference instead. We define the following feature sets. Fr includes only features of class Frequency, while Fr + Lex includes features from both the Frequency and Lexical categories. Full includes all feature sets. All above feature sets take C, the reference corpus for computing F REQUENCY features, to be the entire 58K training samples in both scenarios. In the 4K scenario, we also experiment with FrLim, which includes all features, but takes C to contain only the 4K samples of the training data. We use the Gurobi package for ILP.8 Brown clusters are extracted from the 58K samples of the training data using Liang’s implementation.9 The convex log-likelihood function of G REEDY-L OG L IN is optimized using LBFGS. All features are selected and all parameters are tuned using the development set.

Results

Table 2 presents the results of the three major algorithms in the two main scenarios 58K and 4K. 8 9

Set MI

Table 2: Accuracy of the different models on the test data in

8

β

http://www.gurobi.com http://github.com/percyliang/brown-cluster

30

5

FrLim Fr Fr + Le Full FrLim Fr Fr + Le Full

acc2 MA

55.9 68.7 68.9 68.4 55.1 65.9 66.2 66.3

61.5 75.9 76.2 76.0 60.9 74.2 74.3 74.5

acc3

acc4

E XACT

21.6 40.6 40.7 39.9 20.9 36.0 36.8 36.9

6.9 23.9 23.8 23.1 6.5 19.3 20.4 20.4

8.2 31.7 32.1 31.8 8.2 30.4 30.7 30.4

Table 3: The performance of G LOBAL -P RC on the development set in various settings (4K scenario). Columns correspond to evaluation measures, namely accuracy of sub-sequences of lengths 2 (micro and macro averages), 3 and 4, and exact match. The upper (lower) part of the table presents results for a time limit of β=30 (5). Fr includes the Frequency features estimated on 58K recipes. Fr + Le further includes Lexical features. Full includes all features. FrLim includes all features, where Frequency features are estimated only on 4K recipes.

We find that the structured perceptron algorithm, G LOBAL -P RC, obtains the best results in both cases and under all evaluation measures. The importance of global optimization was also stressed in other works on event ordering (Chambers and Jurafsky, 2008a; Talukdar et al., 2012). In order to assess the contribution of the different components of the model of the best scoring model, G LOBAL -P RC, we compare the performance of the different feature sets and settings of β on the development set in 4K (Table 3). Results reveal the strong impact of the Frequency feature set on the results. Using this category set alone (Fr) yields slightly lower results than using the full feature set, while estimating the Frequency features on a small corpus (FrLim) lowers results dramatically. Adding Lexical and Brown features yields a small improvement over using Frequency alone. While Table 3 demonstrates the importance of β in the performance of G LOBAL -P RC, it also shows that on a limited time budget, a small training set and few features (4K, Fr) and a reasonably small β (5) can yield competitive results. Increasing β from 5 to 30 generally improves results by 2 to 3 percent absolute. The importance of β is further demonstrated in Table 2, where performance with 4K training instances and β = 30 is better than with 58K training instances and β = 5. Preliminary experiments conducted on the development data with higher values of β of 60 and 120 suggest that further increasing β

yields no further improvement. Previous studies evaluated their models on the related problem of distinguishing randomly permuted and correctly ordered chains of events (§2). In this paper we generalize this task to complete event ordering. In order to demonstrate the relative difficulty of the tasks, we apply our highest scoring model (4K, Fr + Le) to the binary task (without re-training it). We do so by computing the percentage of cases in which the correct ordering obtains a higher score than an average ordering. The high resulting accuracy of 93%, as opposed to considerably lower accuracies obtained under ordering evaluation measures, reflects the relative difficulty of the tasks. The proposed edge-factored model can easily capture pair-wise ordering relations between events, but is more limited in accounting for relations between larger sets of events. A Psimple way of doing so is by adding the feature e P (ei |e)P (e|ej ) between events ei and ej (in addition to the regular transition probabilities P (ei |ej )). However, preliminary experimentation with this technique did not yield improved performance. Future work will address higher-order models that straightforwardly account for such long-distance dependencies. To qualitatively assess what generalizations are learned by the model, we apply G LOBAL -P RC to the development data and look at what event pairs obtained either particularly high or particularly low results. For each pair of predicates and their first arguments (a1 ,c11 ), (a2 ,c21 ), we compute the average weight of an edge connecting events of these types, discarding pairs of frequency less than 20. The 20 highest scoring edges contain pairs such as (“add,” “mixing after addition”), (“beat whites,” “fold into mixture”) and (“cover for minutes,” “cook”), in addition to a few noisy pairs resulting from parser errors. The 20 lowest scoring edges contain event pairs that are likely to appear in the opposite order. 11 of the cases include as a first argument the predicates “serve,” “cool” or “chill,” which are likely to occur at the end of a recipe. 3 other edges linked duplications (e.g., (“reduce heat,” “reduce heat”)), which are indeed unlikely to immediately follow one another. These findings suggest the importance of detecting both lexical pairs that are unlikely to follow one another, in addition to those that are likely to.

9

Conclusion

We addressed the problem of lexical event ordering, and developed an edge-factored model for tackling it. We rely on temporally aligned texts, using a new dataset of cooking recipes as a test case, thereby avoiding the need for costly and error-prone manual annotation. We present results of a pair-wise accuracy of over 70% using a basic set of features, and show the utility of the structured perceptron algorithm over simpler greedy and local approaches. The setup we explore, which uses a discriminative model and an ILP formulation, is easy to extend both in terms of features and in terms of more complex formal constraints and edge dependencies, as was done in graph-based dependency parsing (McDonald et al., 2005). Future work will address the extension of the feature set and model, and the application of this model to temporal semantics and planning tasks. We will further address the application of semi-supervised variants of the proposed techniques (e.g., self-training) to other domains, where no sizable corpora of temporally aligned data can be found.

Acknowledgments We would like to thank Nathan Schneider, Roy Schwartz, Bonnie Webber and the members of the Probmodels group at the University of Edinburgh for helpful comments. This work was supported by ERC Advanced Fellowship 249520 GRAMPLUS.

Appendix A: Maximal Hamiltonian Path Let G(S) = (S ∪ {s, f }, E(S)) be an almostcomplete directed graph with E = E(S) = (S ∪ {s}) × (S ∪ {f }). Let cij ∈ R be weights for its edges ((i, j) ∈ E). A Hamiltonian path between s, f ∈ V can be found by solving the following program, returning P = {(i, j)|xij = 1}. max

xij ∈{0,1} : (i,j)∈E ui ∈Z : i∈V

s.t.

n X i=0,i6=j

xij = 1 ∀j 6= s;

n X

cij xij

i6=j n X

xij = 1 ∀i 6= e;

j=0,j6=i

ui − uj + |V |xij ≤ |V | − 1 ∀(i, j) ∈ E

References Niranjan Balasubramanian, Stephen Soderland, Mausam, and Oren Etzioni. 2013. Generating coherent event schemas at scale. In EMNLP ’13, pages 1721–1731. Regina Barzilay and Mirella Lapata. 2008. Modeling local coherence: An entity-based approach. Computational Linguistics, 34(1):1–34. Mario Bollini, Stefanie Tellex, Tyler Thompson, Nicholas Roy, and Daniela Rus. 2013. Interpreting and executing recipes with a cooking robot. In Experimental Robotics, pages 481–495. Springer. Peter F Brown, Peter V Desouza, Robert L Mercer, Vincent J Della Pietra, and Jenifer C Lai. 1992. Classbased n-gram models of natural language. Computational Linguistics, 18(4):467–479. Taylor Cassidy, Bill McDowell, Nathanael Chambers, and Steven Bethard. 2014. An annotation framework for dense event ordering. In ACL ’14: Short Papers, pages 501–506. Nathanael Chambers and Dan Jurafsky. 2008a. Jointly combining implicit constraints improves temporal ordering. In EMNLP ’08, pages 698–706. Nathanael Chambers and Dan Jurafsky. 2008b. Unsupervised learning of narrative event chains. In ACL-HLT ’08, pages 789–797. Nathanael Chambers and Dan Jurafsky. 2009. Unsupervised learning of narrative schemas and their participants. In ACL-IJCNLP ’09, pages 602–610, Suntec, Singapore, August. Timothy Chklovski and Patrick Pantel. 2004. Verbocean: Mining the web for fine-grained semantic verb relations. In EMNLP ’04, pages 33–40. Philipp Cimiano, Janna L¨uker, David Nagel, and Christina Unger. 2013. Exploiting ontology lexica for generating natural language texts from RDF data. In European Workshop on Natural Language Generation, pages 10–19. Michael Collins. 2002. Discriminative training methods for hidden markov models: Theory and experiments with perceptron algorithms. In EMNLP ’02, pages 1– 8. Ido Dagan, Dan Roth, Mark Sammons, and Fabio Massimo Zanzotto. 2013. Recognizing textual entailment: Models and applications. Synthesis Lectures on Human Language Technologies, 6(4):1–220. Dmitry Davidov, Ari Rappoport, and Moshe Koppel. 2007. Fully unsupervised discovery of conceptspecific relationships by web mining. In ACL ’07, pages 232–239. Marie-Catherine de Marneffe and Christopher D Manning. 2008. The Stanford typed dependencies representation. In the COLING ’08 workshop on CrossFramework and Cross-Domain Parser Evaluation, pages 1–8.

Robert M.W. Dixon. 2005. A Semantic Approach To English Grammar. Oxford University Press. Jennifer D’Souza and Vincent Ng. 2013. Classifying temporal relations with rich linguistic knowledge. In NAACL-HLT ’13, pages 918–927. Lea Frermann, Ivan Titov, and Manfred Pinkal. 2014. A hierarchical bayesian model for unsupervised induction of script knowledge. In EACL ’14, pages 49–57. Bram Jans, Steven Bethard, Ivan Vuli´c, and MarieFrancine Moens. 2012. Skip n-grams and ranking functions for predicting script events. In EACL ’12, pages 336–344. Maria Lapata and Alex Lascarides. 2006. Learning sentence-internal temporal relations. Journal of Artificial Intelligence Research (JAIR), 27:85–117. Mirella Lapata. 2003. Probabilistic text structuring: Experiments with sentence ordering. In ACL ’03, pages 545–552. Mirella Lapata. 2006. Automatic evaluation of information ordering: Kendall’s tau. Computational Linguistics, 32(4):471–484. Xiao Ling and Daniel S Weld. 2010. Temporal information extraction. In AAAI ’10, pages 1385 – 1390. Jonathan Malmaud, Earl Wagner, Nancy Chang, and Kevin Murphy. 2014. Cooking with semantics. In the ACL 2014 Workshop on Semantic Parsing, pages 33–38. Inderjeet Mani, Marc Verhagen, Ben Wellner, Chong Min Lee, and James Pustejovsky. 2006. Machine learning of temporal relations. In ACL-COLING ’06, pages 753–760. Mehdi Manshadi, Reid Swanson, and Andrew S. Gordon. 2008. Learning a probabilistic model of event sequences from internet weblog stories. In FLAIRS ’08, pages 159–164. Ryan McDonald, Koby Crammer, and Fernando Pereira. 2005. Online large-margin training of dependency parsers. In ACL ’05, pages 91–98. Paramita Mirza and Sara Tonelli. 2014. Classifying temporal relations with simple features. In EACL ’14, pages 308–317, Gothenburg, Sweden, April. Ashutosh Modi and Ivan Titov. 2014. Inducing neural models of script knowledge. In CoNLL ’14, pages 49– 57. Kira Mour˜ao, Luke Zettlemoyer, Ronald Petrick, and Mark Steedman. 2012. Learning STRIPS operators from noisy and incomplete observations. In UAI ’12. Karl Pichotta and Raymond Mooney. 2014. Statistical script learning with multi-argument events. In EACL ’14, pages 220–229. Emily Pitler and Ani Nenkova. 2009. Using syntax to disambiguate explicit discourse connectives in text. In ACL-IJCNLP ’09: Short Papers, pages 13–16.

James Pustejovsky, Jos´e M Castano, Robert Ingria, Robert J Gaizauskas, Andrea Setzer, Graham Katz, and Dragomir R Radev. 2003. TimeML: Robust specification of event and temporal expressions in text. New directions in question answering, 3:28–34. Michaela Regneri, Alexander Koller, and Manfred Pinkal. 2010. Learning script knowledge with web experiments. In ACL ’10, pages 979–988. Michaela Regneri, Marcus Rohrbach, Dominikus Wetzel, Stefan Thater, Bernt Schiele, and Manfred Pinkal. 2013. Grounding action descriptions in videos. Transactions of the Association for Computational Linguistics (TACL), 1:25–36. Ehud Reiter, Robert Dale, and Zhiwei Feng. 2000. Building natural language generation systems, volume 33. Cambridge: Cambridge university press. Dan Roth and Wen-tau Yih. 2007. Global inference for entity and relation identification via a linear programming formulation. In Lise Getoor and Ben Taskar, editors, Introduction to Statistical Relational Learning. MIT Press. Aju Thalappillil Scaria, Jonathan Berant, Mengqiu Wang, Christopher D Manning, Justin Lewis, Brittany Harding, and Peter Clark. 2013. Learning biological processes with global constraints. In EMNLP ’13. Mark Steedman. 2002. Plans, affordances, and combinatory grammar. Linguistics and Philosophy, 25(56):723–753. Partha Pratim Talukdar, Derry Wijaya, and Tom Mitchell. 2012. Acquiring temporal constraints between relations. In CIKM ’12, pages 992–1001. Dan Tasse and Noah A Smith. 2008. SOUR CREAM: Toward semantic processing of recipes. Technical report, Technical Report CMU-LTI-08-005, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA. Roy Tromble and Jason Eisner. 2009. Learning linear ordering problems for better translation. In EMNLP ’09, pages 1007–1016. Naushad UzZaman, Hector Llorens, Leon Derczynski, James Allen, Marc Verhagen, and James Pustejovsky. 2013. Semeval-2013 task 1: Tempeval-3: Evaluating time expressions, events, and temporal relations. In *SEM-SemEval ’13, pages 1–9. Katsumasa Yoshikawa, Sebastian Riedel, Masayuki Asahara, and Yuji Matsumoto. 2009. Jointly identifying temporal relations with markov logic. In ACL-IJCNLP ’09, pages 405–413.

Loading...

Lexical Event Ordering with an Edge-Factored Model - Informatics

Lexical Event Ordering with an Edge-Factored Model Omri Abend, Shay B. Cohen and Mark Steedman School of Informatics, University of Edinburgh Edinbur...

660KB Sizes 4 Downloads 13 Views

Recommend Documents

Lexical Semantics: An Introduction
The polysemy of a word can be tested by a variety of means, including: * antagonism: can the word be used in a sentence

Event safety - Running an event safely - HSE
Mar 14, 2014 - Guidance on running an event safely. ... For many events, all that's required is a basic series of tasks.

Model Construction with External Constraints: An
Model Construction with External Constraints: An Interactive Journey ... correct with respect to a syntax definition, a

An integrated, interactive and participatory biodiversity informatics
Nov 7, 2016 - shortfall, which limits considerably our ability to support conservation and ... Linnean shortfall , which

Designing an Event Log API with RAML - InfoQ
Apr 11, 2014 - REST API specifications can be very repetitive with a lot of boilerplate and repeating specifications for

Apparel Ordering with Absolute Screen Art
SC MARINERS SWIM INVITATIONAL. Welcome to the SC Mariners Swim Invitational online store. The online store has been deve

Suggest An Event | Hilltown Families
Post your calendar listing HERE, or scroll down to the bottom of this page to find the fields for entering your informat

an event - Mazinaajim Children's Foundation
All proceeds will go to children and youth seeking bursaries for education, recreation and cultural opportunities. All f

Lexical Grammar
like positive/negative, singular/plural, active/passive, and so on. If we look at them carefully, these terms are of cou

LEXICAL STRUCTURE, LEXICAL CONCEPTS AND - Dialnet
dominio emocionológico. Granada: Método Ediciones. Ruiz de Mendoza Ibáñez, F. 1997a. “On “relational frames”: